Intended for healthcare professionals

Papers

Effects of treatment for intestinal helminth infection on growth and cognitive performance in children: systematic review of randomised trials

BMJ 2000; 320 doi: https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.320.7251.1697 (Published 24 June 2000) Cite this as: BMJ 2000;320:1697
  1. Rumona Dickson, lecturer (rdickson{at}liv.ac.uk)a,
  2. Shally Awasthi, associate professor of paediatricsb,
  3. Paula Williamson, lecturerc,
  4. Colin Demellweek, lecturerd,
  5. Paul Garner, senior lecturera
  1. a International Health Division, Liverpool School of Tropical Medicine, Liverpool L3 5QA
  2. b Department of Paediatrics and Epidemiology, King George Medical College, Lucknow, India
  3. c Department of Mathematical Sciences, University of Liverpool, Liverpool L69 3BX
  4. d Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Liverpool
  1. Correspondence to: R Dickson
  • Accepted 16 March 2000

Abstract

Objective: To summarise the effects of anthelmintic drug treatment on growth and cognitive performance in children.

Data sources: Electronic databases: Cochrane Infectious Diseases Group controlled trial register, Cochrane controlled trials register, Embase, and Medline. Citations of all identified trials. Contact with the World Health Organization and field researchers.

Review methods: Systematic review of randomised controlled trials in children aged 1–16 that compared anthelmintic treatment with placebo or no treatment. Assessment of validity and data abstraction conducted independently by two reviewers.

Main outcome measures: Growth and cognitive performance.

Results: Thirty randomised controlled trials in more than 15 000 children were identified. Effects on mean weight were unremarkable, and heterogeneity was evident in the results. There were some positive effects on mean weight change in the trials reporting this outcome: after a single dose (any anthelmintic) the pooled estimates were 0.24 kg (95% confidence interval 0.15 kg to 0.32 kg; fixed effects model assumed) and 0.38 kg (0.01 kg to 0.77 kg; random effects model assumed). Results from trials of multiple doses showed mean weight change in up to one year of follow up of 0.10 kg (0.04 kg to 0.17 kg; fixed effects) or 0.15 kg (0.00 to 0.30; random effects). At more than one year of follow up, mean weight change was 0.12 kg (−0.02 kg to 0.26 kg; fixed effects) and 0.43 (−0.61 to 1.47; random effects). Results from studies of cognitive performance were inconclusive.

Conclusions: There is some limited evidence that routine treatment of children in areas where helminths are common has effects on weight gain, but this is not consistent between trials. There is insufficient evidence as to whether this intervention improves cognitive performance.

Introduction

One third of the world's population is infected with one or more species of intestinal helminth,1 and public health specialists are concerned that these infections impair children's growth and development. Studies have shown associations between helminth infection and undernutrition, iron deficiency anaemia, stunted growth, poor school attendance, and poor performance in cognition tests.25 Better sanitation reduces transmission, but another approach is to treat children or whole populations routinely to reduce infection rates. Whether this is a sustainable solution is not clear, as rapid reinfection occurs.6 Reports of successful combined programmes spanning several decades have come from Japan.7

The World Bank claims that worm infections impair learning and that helminth control is one of the most cost effective strategies to improve health in developing countries.8 Both the World Bank and World Health Organization (WHO) promote helminth control programmes in developing countries as a cost effective intervention.9 Programmes aim to “target mass treatment of children,” giving all children in communities where worms are endemic anthelmintic drugs every three to six months. Treatment regimens depend on local prevalence rates.6 812 Although studies have shown that available drugs are effective in decreasing parasite infection rates, it is not clear if these approaches actually improve the growth and cognitive performance of children.13 14 We summarised the available evidence from trials of effects of anthelmintic drug programmes on growth and cognitive performance in children.

Methods

Inclusion criteria

We included all randomised and quasirandomised controlled trials in children aged 1–16 years, allocated to either anthelmintic drug treatment for intestinal nematodes with any anthelminth drug (mebendazole, piperazine, albendazole, levamisole, pyrantel, thiabendazole, bephenium, tetrachlorethylene, metronidazole, and ivermectin) or placebo (or no treatment). We required trials to report outcomes related to growth or cognitive performance, and these were our primary outcomes. We had no restrictions on language, publication status, or where the intervention was dispensed.

Identification of trials

We searched the controlled trial register of the Cochrane Infectious Diseases Group, the Cochrane controlled trials register, Embase, and Medline using terms related to specific infections and specific common drugs. WHO and field researchers were contacted for unpublished or ongoing trials. Citations of all trials identified in the search were checked for further references.

Study selection and data extraction

Two reviewers independently carried out study selection and assessment of study quality (RD and PW). Data on growth outcomes were extracted independently by two reviewers using previously designed data extraction tables (RD and SA). Data related to cognitive performance were extracted by one reviewer (CD) and checked by a second reviewer (RD). Assessment of trial quality included evaluation of concealment of allocation, method of randomisation, method of analysis, and loss to follow up.

Statistical analysis

For each continuous outcome specified we calculated a pooled estimate of the weighted mean difference across the studies and applied a χ2 test for homogeneity. Analyses of results from single and multiple dose trials in the short term (less than one year) and long term (over one year) were undertaken separately. Within these categories analysis was carried out to compare each drug with placebo or no treatment independently and overall. When there was significant heterogeneity both fixed and random effects estimates are given. We explored heterogeneity by type of drug, age of children, and intensity of infection.

Description of studies

Thirty trials (reported in 36 reports) met the inclusion criteria (details can be found on the BMJ website).1347 Of these, five (reported in seven reports 18 24 30 33 3537) reported cognitive outcomes, while two reports from one study included both cognitive and growth outcomes.45 46 Two trials that reported effects on growth or nutrition compared the effectiveness of different drug dosages or drugs and are not included in this review.48 49 Twenty eight trials were published reports, one was a report drafted in 1997 (S Awasthi, unpublished data), and one was an unpublished report of preliminary data from an ongoing trial.16 The results of all included trials were available in English. The trials were carried out in 17 countries in four continents (Africa, Asia, Central America, and South America). Drugs assessed in these trials included albendazole, piperazine, mebendazole, levamisole, pyrantel, and tetrachlorethylene. Follow up periods for single dose trials were from four weeks to one year (15 trials) and for multiple dose trials were from 26 weeks to six years (15 trials).

Children included in the trials were recruited from school populations in 17 trials, rural communities in nine trials, and through health clinics or health workers in four trials. Twenty seven of the included trials used “targeted mass treatment”—that is, all children were treated regardless of infection status.1 The other three trials used “targeted screening,” in which children were screened and only infected children were eligible for inclusion.

Overall quality of the trials was poor. Of the 30 included studies, only three adequately reported concealed allocation, and 24 made no mention of concealment of allocation. A further three trials mentioned that the list of participants was sorted according to age, sex, or intensity of infection and then randomly allocated to treatment or control groups. Four trials specified the method of randomisation. Six trials used cluster randomisation,16 21 24 34 42 44 but only two seem to have adjusted for design effects in their analysis.16 42

Twenty eight trials seemed to use an intention to treat analysis in so far as they reported children being analysed in the groups to which they had been randomised. One trial regrouped children after randomisation according to the number of drug treatments actually received.20 One trial excluded tall children from their analysis as the results from these children exceeded the upper limits of the comparison reference values being used.32

Analysis of different outcomes and at different times made a single estimate of loss to follow up problematic. As a general rule, loss to follow up was calculated on the basis of measurements at the final testing time. In five studies the degree of loss to follow up was unclear.21 22 25 27 30 Eight studies had a loss to follow up of less than 10%, while in 15 it was between 10% and 35%. One study had loss to follow up of 40%,20 while a second failed to collect data on more than 60% of study participants.44

Reporting of growth data varied. Some trials reported actual weight or height at the end of follow up, while others reported these variables as change in weight or height, weight for height, or weight for age. Some authors reported only a selected subset of the data and data analysis.21 Selective reporting is a concern in this review as it is unclear whether various growth outcomes were analysed but only selected findings reported.

One study acknowledged financial support for the trial from a drug company. Four stated they received the drugs used in the trial from the manufacturer.

Results

Growth

Eleven trials evaluated a single dose treatment and reported growth outcomes; two studies reported results from single and multiple drug doses (S Awasthi, unpublished data).16 An additional four multiple dose trials recorded data after their first treatment dose, and these data were included in the single dose analysis.19 22 40 45 A subsequent sensitivity analysis that excluded data from these four multiple dose trials did not show any change in effect trends. Follow up varied from four weeks to one year.

Five single dose studies that included data on growth did not provide the data in a manner that allowed for them to be included in the meta-analysis. Four of these showed no differences in growth measures between groups,13 17 27 31 while one showed a difference in favour of the treatment group.28 All of the tables of results are on the BMJ's website. In summary, for single dose trials the pooled estimates of increase in weight were 0.24 kg (95% confidence interval 0.15 kg to 0.32 kg; fixed effects model) and 0.38 kg (0.01 kg to 0.77 kg; random effects model). For multiple dose trials the figures were 0.10 kg (0.04 kg to 0.17 kg; fixed effects) and 0.15 kg (0.00 to 0.30; random effects) for up to one year of follow up and 0.12 kg (−0.02 kg to 0.26 kg; fixed effects) and 0.43 (−0.61 to 1.47; random effects) for more than one year of follow up.

In studies that provided data that could be used in the meta-analysis there were significant differences in relation to mean changes in weight, height, mid-upper arm circumference, triceps and subscapular skinfold measures, and mean triceps and subscapular skinfold measures.

Fourteen trials reported on the effectiveness of multiple doses with outcomes measured at one year or less. Seven trials provided data that could be used in the meta-analysis. There were significant differences in mean change in weight and in mid-upper arm circumference, and mean and mean change in triceps and subscapular skinfold measures. Seven of the 14 trials did not provide data that could be used in the meta-analysis. Of these, two trials of mebendazole found no differences in the groups.14 34 Two studies that assessed levamisole reported increases in weight and height 44 47; however, the trial reported by Thein et al had a loss to follow up of over 60%.44 One trial that used pyrantel or albendazole found no differences but was confounded by a drop in rates of parasitism in control children.26 Fernando et al reported only selected data, from which no conclusions could be drawn.21 Evans et al did not carry out an intention to treat analysis because of crossovers in the trial but found that children who received treatment with pyrantel had increases in growth measures and a decrease in morbidity as measured by the development of measles.20

Results from the combination of two trials that lasted more than one year and used albendazole showed no difference between treatment and placebo groups for measures of mean weight, and the mean weight change of 120 g was not significant (S Awasthi, unpublished data).16

Cognitive performance

Five studies reported on the effects of treatment on measures of cognitive ability. Two studies that used albendazole attempted to assess the effects of anthelmintic treatment on academic progress. Watkins et al examined reading vocabulary and attendance records of 228 students treated with albendazole or placebo and found no difference in the groups at six months.46 Simeon et al examined performance on reading, spelling, and arithmetic tests, as well as school attendance, in 407 students and found no difference between treated and untreated children positive for trichuris.35 The loss of 35% of the school attendance records makes the interpretation of the results related to school performance difficult. This research was reported in three separate papers. Two of these studies examined the effects of anthelmintic treatment in different subgroups of children by using overlapping but differing batteries of tests of cognitive ability.36 37 Neither study found a significant effect of treatment.

Nokes et al found some beneficial effect on some aspects of cognitive functioning.32 33 Simeon et al failed to find this benefit, although multiple regression analysis found that children with heavy infections showed more improvement than those with lighter infections.35 Boivin and Giordani found that anthelmintic treatment with levamisole combined with iron supplementation had beneficial effects on cognitive function, but they could not separate the effects of the two treatments.18 In one other study, in which mebendazole was not effective in significantly decreasing helminth prevalence, there were no consistent changes in cognitive performance after treatment,30 but the authors attributed the lack of effect to the failure of drug treatment. They consequently attempted to replicate their study by using treatment specific for helminth infection. Unfortunately a natural disaster (flood) precluded data collection in the second study. Hadidjaja et al compared the use of mebendazole alone or in combination with health education.24 Their groups, however, were not comparable at inception, and loss to follow up was over 30%, with losses in some groups over 50%. All studies that examined effects of treatment on cognitive performance also measured growth factors such as height and weight. Only one study, however, provided data in a form that could be used in the meta-analysis.45 46

Discussion

The main question examined in this review was the effect of anthelmintics on growth and cognitive performance. Therefore we did not examine drug effects on intermediate outcomes such as worm infection. Head to head comparisons of different drugs are not included as these are not relevant until benefits in placebo controlled comparisons have been shown. We did not consider intermediate laboratory variables such as haemoglobin concentration. In fact, of the trials that met our inclusion criteria only three reported this outcome in a manner that could be used in the meta-analysis, and the evidence of an effect is limited.50

We intended to analyse effects by prespecified factors that could influence these estimates, including the presence of malnutrition, infection species, and intensity of infection. We also intended to stratify our analysis by age, as this will strongly influence the size of many growth outcomes. The data available from individual trials, however, were insufficient to allow subgroup analysis or meta-regression by any of these factors. Analysis of data from individual patients might start to unravel these subgroup effects but would be a large task.

Not unexpectedly, and probably related to the factors mentioned above, several meta-analyses showed heterogeneity between the results from different trials. We considered presenting only those results with no heterogeneity. Most growth outcomes are correlated within patients, however, so we judged that reporting only those meta-analyses where heterogeneity was absent would be a bias in itself. For this reason, we present all meta-analyses, and, where there was significant heterogeneity, we have provided analysis using both fixed and random effects models. The wide confidence intervals with random effects reflect the heterogeneity in the available data. Full details of individual trial data are available from the Cochrane Library.50

The trials

The analysis highlighted several methodological issues that should be examined in future trials. Firstly, trial quality varied. Most trials were small, and few reported that they concealed allocation.

Secondly, most researchers obtained multiple growth measures, but their reporting of these was inconsistent. Authors variously reported absolute values, changes in values, indices of values (such as weight for height, height for age), and comparison of values with accepted international reference data. Researchers did not indicate their a priori primary outcome. We were therefore unable to assess whether the data presented were the intended primary outcomes or whether a number of growth outcomes were measured with the authors reporting only on significant results. This bias, termed selective reporting, may be done unwittingly by researchers but is potentially an important unresearched source of bias in systematic reviews of published literature.

Thirdly, growth is an indirect measure of health outcome, and only one study examined more direct measures such as illness or mortality. Evans et al attempted to examine morbidity measures such as episodes of diarrhoea, respiratory infection, or measles.20 The results of their study indicated that there were fewer episodes of measles in treated children.

Finally, policy makers assume that these interventions improve school attendance, and we specifically sought this outcome. Two studies measured this, and neither found an effect. Other studies used cognitive tests, but what these mean and how they relate to a child's wellbeing is not clear. Researchers used a wide range of tests, which made direct comparison difficult. Even when the same test was used, such as the digit span (forwards) and verbal fluency test, the effects were not consistent between studies. Again, the hypothesis that routine anthelmintic treatment will benefit a subgroup of the population could not be explored with the data available to us. A further difficulty is that most tests for cognitive performance have not been validated outside Western countries.51 52

On the other hand, the real problems in implementing these trials should not be underestimated, and difficulties can result in contamination, which mitigates against the detection of true effect differences between groups. In the study by Evans et al, in which fortnightly assessments were carried out, the researchers found that a proportion (>30%) of children in the placebo group had actually received intermittent anthelmintic treatment.20 The reported crossover rate in the Awasthi trial further indicates the difficulty in maintaining treatment groups within a study population (S Awasthi, unpublished data).

Policy implications

Our interpretation of these findings is that the evidence of benefit for mass treatment of children related to positive effects on growth and cognitive performance is not convincing. In the light of these data, we would be unwilling to recommend that countries or regions invest in programmes that routinely treat children with anthelmintic drugs to improve their growth or cognitive performance.

Research implications

There is a need for good quality, properly concealed, placebo controlled trials to investigate the impact on these outcomes. It is inefficient, unjustified, and irrelevant to carry out head to head comparisons of different anthelmintic drugs. A large cluster, randomised trial in India is currently examining mortality as an outcome, and the results of this work will be an important contribution. International coordination, possibly through the WHO, is required to ensure that future trials are similar in design and that researchers agree in advance to pool individual patients' data in a meta-analysis and to explore hypotheses about subgroup effects in relation to age, worm load, and exposure to infection.

What is already known on this topic

Many children in developing countries are infected by intestinal helminths and infection is associated with poor nutrition

In endemic areas the World Bank and others widely promote routine regular mass treatment of children with anthelmintics

What this study adds

There is little evidence to support the use of routine anthelmintic treatment to improve growth and cognitive performance in children in developing countries

Acknowledgments

We thank the advisory panel for this review (Dr M Albonico, Dr G Barnish, Dr D Bundy, and Dr L Savioli) and Dr P Donnen for his prompt responses to our requests for data. A more detailed version of this review is published on the Cochrane Library and will be maintained as new evidence emerges.

Contributors: RD wrote the protocol, assisted all aspects of the review process, organised specialist advice and input, drafted the review, and organised its final production. SA assisted in assessment of study inclusion and data extraction. PW assisted in assessment of study quality and data analysis. CD extracted cognitive data and interpreted the results. PG conceived the idea, guided the protocol development, contributed to the data interpretation, and provided support in the preparation of both the Cochrane review and this manuscript. All authors commented on drafts of this publication. PG is guarantor.

Footnotes

  • Funding Department for International Development UK and the European Union Directorate General XII. The panel and the funding agencies have no responsibility for the data presented and the views expressed.

  • Competing interests SA is currently supported by Unicef and the World Bank to carry out a trial of anthelmintic drugs in children. No author is currently receiving support from drug companies manufacturing anthelmintics.

  • Embedded Image The tables of results are available on the BMJ website

References

  1. 1.
  2. 2.
  3. 3.
  4. 4.
  5. 5.
  6. 6.
  7. 7.
  8. 8.
  9. 9.
  10. 10.
  11. 11.
  12. 12.
  13. 13.
  14. 14.
  15. 15.
  16. 16.
  17. 17.
  18. 18.
  19. 19.
  20. 20.
  21. 21.
  22. 22.
  23. 23.
  24. 24.
  25. 25.
  26. 26.
  27. 27.
  28. 28.
  29. 29.
  30. 30.
  31. 31.
  32. 32.
  33. 33.
  34. 34.
  35. 35.
  36. 36.
  37. 37.
  38. 38.
  39. 39.
  40. 40.
  41. 41.
  42. 42.
  43. 43.
  44. 44.
  45. 45.
  46. 46.
  47. 47.
  48. 48.
  49. 49.
  50. 50.
  51. 51.
  52. 52.