Intended for healthcare professionals

CCBYNC Open access
Research Christmas 2017: The Lives of Doctors

Efficacy of educational video game versus traditional educational apps at improving physician decision making in trauma triage: randomized controlled trial

BMJ 2017; 359 doi: https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.j5416 (Published 13 December 2017) Cite this as: BMJ 2017;359:j5416
  1. Deepika Mohan, assistant professor of critical care medicine1,
  2. Coreen Farris, behavioral scientist2,
  3. Baruch Fischhoff, professor of engineering and public policy3,
  4. Matthew R Rosengart, professor of surgery4,
  5. Derek C Angus, professor and chair of critical care medicine1,
  6. Donald M Yealy, professor and chair of emergency medicine5,
  7. David J Wallace, assistant professor of critical care medicine1,
  8. Amber E Barnato, professor in health care delivery6
  1. 1Scaife Hall, 3550 Terrace St, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15261, USA
  2. 24570 Fifth Avenue, Suite 600, RAND Corporation, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
  3. 3Porter Hall 219E, 5000 Forbes Avenue, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
  4. 4F1266 Presbyterian Hospital, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, 15213, USA
  5. 53600 Meyran Avenue, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, USA
  6. 6The Dartmouth Institute, Williamson Translational Building, 5th Floor, One Medical Center Drive, Lebanon, NH 03756, USA
  1. Correspondence to: D Mohan mohand{at}upmc.edu

Abstract

Objective To determine whether a behavioral intervention delivered through a video game can improve the appropriateness of trauma triage decisions in the emergency department of non-trauma centers.

Design Randomized clinical trial.

Setting Online intervention in national sample of emergency medicine physicians who make triage decisions at US hospitals.

Participants 368 emergency medicine physicians primarily working at non-trauma centers. A random sample (n=200) of those with primary outcome data was reassessed at six months.

Interventions Physicians were randomized in a 1:1 ratio to one hour of exposure to an adventure video game (Night Shift) or apps based on traditional didactic education (myATLS and Trauma Life Support MCQ Review), both on iPads. Night Shift was developed to recalibrate the process of using pattern recognition to recognize moderate-severe injuries (representativeness heuristics) through the use of stories to promote behavior change (narrative engagement). Physicians were randomized with a 2×2 factorial design to intervention (game v traditional education apps) and then to the experimental condition under which they completed the outcome assessment tool (low v high cognitive load). Blinding could not be maintained after allocation but group assignment was masked during the analysis phase.

Main outcome measures Outcomes of a virtual simulation that included 10 cases; in four of these the patients had severe injuries. Participants completed the simulation within four weeks of their intervention. Decisions to admit, discharge, or transfer were measured. The proportion of patients under-triaged (patients with severe injuries not transferred to a trauma center) was calculated then (primary outcome) and again six months later, with a different set of cases (primary outcome of follow-up study). The secondary outcome was effect of cognitive load on under-triage.

Results 149 (81%) physicians in the game arm and 148 (80%) in the traditional education arm completed the trial. Of these, 64/100 (64%) and 58/100 (58%), respectively, completed reassessment at six months. The mean age was 40 (SD 8.9), 283 (96%) were trained in emergency medicine, and 207 (70%) were ATLS (advanced trauma life support) certified. Physicians exposed to the game under-triaged fewer severely injured patients than those exposed to didactic education (316/596 (0.53) v 377/592 (0.64), estimated difference 0.11, 95% confidence interval 0.05 to 0.16; P<0.001). Cognitive load did not influence under-triage (161/308 (0.53) v 155/288 (0.54) in the game arm; 197/300 (0.66) v 180/292 (0.62) in the traditional educational apps arm; P=0.66). At six months, physicians exposed to the game remained less likely to under-triage patients (146/256 (0.57) v 172/232 (0.74), estimated difference 0.17, 0.09 to 0.25; P<0.001). No physician reported side effects. The sample might not reflect all emergency medicine physicians, and a small set of cases was used to assess performance.

Conclusions Compared with apps based on traditional didactic education, exposure of physicians to a theoretically grounded video game improved triage decision making in a validated virtual simulation. Though the observed effect was large, the wide confidence intervals include the possibility of a small benefit, and the real world efficacy of this intervention remains uncertain.

Trial registration clinicaltrials.gov; NCT02857348 (initial study)/NCT03138304 (follow-up).

Introduction

Medical diagnosis often requires physicians to collect and integrate complex uncertain information from multiple sources.1 Under normal conditions, that process requires reliance on heuristic cognitive processes.23 Heuristics generate solutions to complex problems through pattern recognition and simplifying assumptions. When calibrated well, heuristics allow people to function under conditions of time pressure and uncertainty.4 When calibrated poorly, however, they result in predictable errors in judgment.5 As a result, many interventions attempt to reduce reliance on heuristic cognitive processes.6 Some of these interventions focus on increasing physicians’ use of clinical practice guidelines through direct instruction (such as warning about the risks of relying on heuristics, checklists of necessary actions) or outcome feedback (such as telling physicians how they have done).7891011 Others try to shift the locus of decision making from the bedside clinician to a third party such as a decision tool or an external consultant.1213 To date, these interventions have had mixed success, with limited transference across task domains.6 More importantly, they do not directly deal with the long term need to improve the heuristic processes that underpin most physician decision making.1141516

Behavioral scientists agree that people develop well calibrated heuristics when the decisional context provides reliable valid cues to the problem and they have the opportunity to learn the relevant contextual cues.17 Typically, calibration—the process of refining the accuracy of judgment—requires an experience-feedback loop. Researchers in other specialties (such as aviation, organizational science, threat detection), however, have experimented with different behavioral interventions that serve as surrogates for this process.181920 We created such an intervention for a medical diagnosis task that has proved difficult to improve: trauma triage decisions.

Trauma triage involves the identification and transfer of severely injured patients to trauma centers, either directly from the field or after evaluation at a non-trauma center. High levels of under-triage (the failure to transfer severely injured patients to trauma centers) persist, despite efforts to improve best practice. The problem is particularly acute at non-trauma centers, where fewer than 30% of severely injured patients are transferred as recommended by clinical practice guidelines.2122232425 Our prior experimental and observational work suggests that heuristics play an important role in under-triage.2627 We selected one promising method of recalibrating heuristics—narrative engagement—and developed a theoretically grounded intervention delivered through the platform of a video game. Narrative engagement is defined as the use of compelling stories to communicate and encode principles of best practice decision.

We compared the efficacy of a narrative engagement video game with that of a prominent education intervention for improving simulated decisions in trauma triage with emergency medicine physicians practicing at non-trauma centers as participants.

Methods

Overview

We have previously published the study protocol for this trial.28 We developed a video game (Night Shift) in collaboration with Schell Games (Pittsburgh, PA) and conducted a randomized controlled trial of the effect of the game compared with traditional didactic education, administered through commercially available applications, on triage by US emergency physicians practicing at non-trauma centers. We hypothesized that physicians exposed to game based education would under-triage fewer patients on a virtual simulation than those exposed to the didactic education program (primary trial outcome) and that experimentally induced cognitive load would degrade triage performance less among physicians exposed to the game than among those exposed to the didactic program (secondary trial outcome). Process measures included adherence to the interventions, as well as their usability and likeability.

Subsequently, we assessed the duration of the treatment effect among a random sample of those who completed the outcome assessment tool by measuring physician performance again six months after the completion of the initial trial protocol (primary six month reassessment outcome). We hypothesized that physicians exposed to game based education during the initial trial would continue to under-triage fewer patients on the simulation than physicians exposed to the didactic program.

Conceptual model

In our conceptual model, grounded in behavioral decision research, physicians first judge the severity of the injury and then decide how to manage it.329 Those judgments reflect the interaction between what are called system one processes, which are fast, automatic, and heuristic, and system two processes, which are slow, deliberate, and analytic.2 Under time pressure, people increasingly default to heuristics (or mental shortcuts) that can produce accurate answers but also predictable errors.30 As time pressure decreases, people are better able to synthesize the complex uncertain elements of difficult decisions—assuming that they have the training to do so.2 The decisions themselves also reflect the influence of other variables.31 In triage, these could include physicians’ attitudes towards guidelines, institutional norms, resource constraints, and patient preferences.

Our intervention sought to modify system one processes so as to improve heuristic thinking in decision making in trauma triage. Our specific strategy was based on clinical experience and experimental observations.27 For example, we observed that patients with gunshot wounds were far more likely to be transferred to a trauma center than patients who had fallen, even when they had similar injury severity scores. That pattern is consistent with judgment by representativeness: physicians have an archetype (a pattern) of how severely injured patients present and then transfer patients who match (“representative” cases) but admit or discharge those who do not (“non-representative” cases). Crucially, representativeness does not depend on how often a case occurs but reflects an associative judgment, informed by experience and training.32 As a result, physicians can systematically make correct decisions for cases they deem “obvious” (representative) but make errors with those that are less obvious (non-representative), regardless of frequency.3 Furthermore, physicians with different backgrounds (such as trauma surgeons versus emergency medicine physicians) might make different judgments by representativeness. Heuristics are well calibrated if they align with the reference standard for the specific decision context. We developed Night Shift to recalibrate the heuristics of emergency medicine physicians in trauma triage.

Participants

The triage of trauma patients by physicians occurs at non-trauma centers and level III/IV centers. The designated trauma level of a hospital reflects its ability to manage injuries definitively, based on an accreditation process conducted by the American College of Surgeons and state authorities. The scale ranges from I (fully resourced hospital; serves as regional referral center) to IV (minimally resourced hospital; capable of stabilizing patients but must refer severe injuries to a higher level of care). Hospitals that have not applied for accreditation are referred to as non-trauma centers. Our goal was to recruit a national sample of emergency medicine physicians who make triage decisions. To that end, we recruited board ertified and board eligible physicians working primarily outside level I/II trauma centers in the US at the 2016 annual scientific meeting of the American College of Emergency Physicians (October 16-18).

We randomized physicians using a 2×2 factorial design, with a 1:1:1:1 allocation to complete either game based or didactic educational applications and to complete the outcome assessment tool under conditions of low or high cognitive load. We anticipated that variation in the cognitive load would amplify the effect of heuristics on performance, thereby allowing us to isolate the mechanism by which the game influenced performance.30 Our randomization scheme was generated in Stata 13.0 with block sizes of four and eight. After participants registered, study personnel obtained their assignment to the intervention and outcome assessment condition from a central database. Although we could not maintain blindness after allocation, we masked group assignment during the analysis phase.

Study protocol

At enrollment, physicians received an iPad mini 2 loaded with their intervention. We asked them to spend at least an hour with the intervention and then log onto a secure website that hosted a questionnaire to assess demographics and personal characteristics; a questionnaire to assess use of the interventions (adherence, usability, likeability); and a virtual simulation that served as the outcome assessment tool. Responding to the questionnaires and simulation took about 60 minutes. Participants completed the protocol at their convenience and could keep the iPad (worth about $260 (£195, €218)). They received weekly email reminders until the study closed on 14 November 2016 or until they completed the protocol.

Six months after the completion of the initial trial (May 2017), we emailed a random sample of 100 physicians from each group who had completed all primary study procedures to ask if they would participate in a second assessment. We asked respondents to complete the outcome assessment tool a second time (with a different set of cases) and offered a $100 Amazon gift card on completion of the task. Physicians who agreed to participate received weekly email reminders until the study closed on 15 June 2017.

Questionnaire to assess demographics

Each physician completed a questionnaire with items on age, sex, race, ethnicity, educational background (board certification, ATLS (advanced trauma life support) certification, years since completion of residency), and practice environment (trauma designation of their hospital, affiliation with a level I/II trauma center, affiliation with an emergency medicine residency program). We used the Big Five Inventory-10 for an exploratory analysis of personality traits that might influence the efficacy of the different interventions.33

Questionnaire to assess physicians’ use of interventions

To measure adherence, we asked physicians to report how long they spent using their interventions. To measure the usability and likeability of the interventions, we asked physicians to provide qualitative feedback about the experience.

Game based education: Night Shift

Based on the input of an expert panel of seven trauma surgeons, we distilled the clinical practice guidelines for the triage of trauma patients into three simple principles. The following types of patients have severe injuries until proved otherwise: elderly (>70) and frail patients; patients with injuries affecting more than one body region; and patients with rib fractures or open long bone fractures. We built Night Shift, a two dimensional adventure video game that relies on narrative engagement (that is, the use of compelling stories to promote behavior change) to disseminate these principles. Three research threads support the potential for narrative engagement to alter judgment. One research thread finds that stories facilitate the processing and retention of new data.3435 The second body of research finds that practicing desired behaviors in a safe environment helps people to gain warranted feelings of self efficacy, providing the confidence needed to deploy newly acquired skills.36 The third body of research finds that stories can engage players cognitively and emotionally in ways that transcend traditional forms of education.37

Players take on the persona of Andy Jordan, a young emergency medicine physician who moves home after the disappearance of his estranged grandfather and takes a job in the local emergency department. They are given the dual objective of solving the mystery of the grandfather’s absence and of interacting with the patients who present to the department. These patients have various traumatic and non-traumatic complaints, ranging from the obscure (such as foul smelling body odor and fever after exposure to camel’s milk) to the common (such as low speed motor vehicle collision with minimal injuries). The game centers on a series of trauma patients who arrive with “non-representative” severe injuries—cases in which the injury complex does not fit the popular archetype for the problem. As players interact with these patients, they gain experience with the consequences of under-triage. Specifically, these patients return with complications from their injury. Not only do players have to find a solution for the patient’s deteriorating clinical condition, they also have to explain the outcome to characters in the game (such as family members, consultants). In their responses, the characters provide didactic information about relevant contextual cues for the evaluation of the trauma patients. At the same time, they highlight the repercussions of under-triage (such as preventable disability from a delay in treatment) to evoke an emotional response that would make the feedback memorable (fig 1).

Fig 1
Fig 1

Selected screenshots from interaction between Andy and his boss, the department chair. In this instance, Andy has failed to transfer a patient (Benjamin) with a cervical spine fracture to a trauma center, and Benjamin has returned with a central cord syndrome. During the conversation, players can choose how they want to respond to the department chair’s criticism of their performance, either accepting responsibility or arguing that the complication represents the natural evolution of the disease process rather than a diagnostic error

We made three additional design decisions to enhance players’ emotional and cognitive engagement with the game. First, we embedded the medical component of the game within an overarching mystery. During the quest to find Andy’s grandfather, the player uncovers Andy’s background and motivation. The process allows the player to gain empathy for Andy, which in turn makes the feedback provided by characters in the game feel personally relevant. Second, we included patients with “representative” severe injuries—injuries that do fit the popular archetype of the problem—in the mix of cases that arrive at the emergency department. These patients decompensate shortly after arrival. To salvage them, players have to participate in various team based resuscitation scenarios. In play tests, we found that even a small amount of structured role playing embedded in the adventure increased immersion in the story. Third, we incorporated a puzzle solving mechanic into the medical portion of the game to increase its cognitive challenge. We based the non-trauma patients on abstracted versions of clinical case challenges published in the New England Journal of Medicine.3839404142 Not all the relevant information is provided, forcing players to draw connections between associated pieces of data. Only if they make the right connections, do patients offer the information required to make the correct diagnosis, allowing the player to initiate the correct treatment.

Didactic education apps: myATLS and Trauma Life Support MCQ Review

The ideal standard educational strategy in trauma is Advanced Trauma Life Support (ATLS)—a two day seminar designed to teach participants to resuscitate and stabilize trauma patients and to determine if the patient’s needs exceeds the capabilities of their facility.43 Participants must complete a multiple choice test before and after the course to receive certification. As a surrogate, we provided physicians with two educational software apps: myATLS and Trauma Life Support MCQ Review. The former contains a summary of all the content provided in the ATLS course.44 The latter is designed to help users prepare for the ATLS exam and contains 550 multiple choice questions. We asked participants to use their discretion in deciding how to allocate the hour they spent reviewing the two apps.

Outcome assessment

We developed a virtual simulation to provide a high fidelity replication of the emergency department environment so that we could assess trauma triage decision making in a controlled environment.45 When designing the simulation, we designated certain cases as “representative” (fitting the popular archetype for severe or minor injuries) and others as “non-representative” (not fitting the popular archetype for severe or minor injuries) based on clinical experience and experimental observations. We previously established the simulations’ internal reliability and construct validity. In prior research, we found that physicians, as a group, make similar diagnostic (such as acquisition of computed tomogram) and triage decisions (such as transfer) for trauma patients on the simulation as in clinical practice.45

The simulation presents 10 cases over 42 minutes, representing a busy eight hour shift. It includes four patients with severe injuries, two with minimal injuries, and four with non-traumatic complaints (see appendix for details). Each case includes a 2D rendering of the patient, a chief complaint, vital signs that update every 30 seconds, a history, and a written description of the physical exam (fig 2). Users evaluate and manage patients by selecting from a prespecified list of 250 medications, studies, and procedures. Some orders affect a patient’s clinical status, leading to corresponding changes in their vital signs and findings on physical exam. For example, a blood transfusion given to a patient in hemorrhagic shock will stabilize his/her blood pressure. Other orders generate additional information, presented as reports added to the patients’ charts. The cases end when physicians either make a disposition decision (admit, discharge, transfer) or the patient dies.

Fig 2
Fig 2

Screenshots from the virtual simulation (outcome assessment tool). A) Each case included a 2D rendering of the patient, a chief complaint, vital signs, a history, and a written description of the physical exam. B) Physicians could select from a prespecified list of 250 medications, studies, and procedures. C) Audiovisual distractors were included, such as nursing requests for help with disruptive patients, to increase the verisimilitude of the experience

New patients arrive at prespecified (but unpredictable) intervals so that physicians manage multiple patients concurrently. In addition to their clinical responsibilities, participants also have to respond to various audiovisual distractors, including nursing requests for help with disruptive patients, interruptions by families asking for information, and paging alerts.

During the initial study, we randomized physicians to complete the simulation under conditions of low or high cognitive load to test the mechanism of the treatment effect. We manipulated cognitive load in two ways. First, we varied the complexity of the non-trauma cases. In the low load arm, non-trauma patients had routine complaints (such as appendicitis), arrived hemodynamically stable, and did not deteriorate over the course of the simulation. In the high load arm, non-trauma patients were critically ill (such as septic), arrived hemodynamically unstable, and deteriorated without adequate management. Second, we reduced the number of rooms that physicians could use to evaluate patients from eight in the low load arm to four in the high load arm, which increased the number of distractors that they received.

Our primary outcome for the trial was physicians’ performance on the simulation as measured by the proportion of under-triaged severely injured patients. The secondary outcome of the trial was the effect of cognitive load on these simulated triage decisions.

Assessment of duration of treatment effect

Physicians who participated in the follow-up study completed the virtual simulation a second time after six months, with a different set of trauma cases (see appendix). Given the limited cohort size, we standardized the cognitive load manipulation for all participants, exposing all participants to high load conditions. The primary outcome of this follow-up study was the proportion of severely injured patients under-triaged.

Analyses

We calculated the response rate as the proportion of enrolled physicians who logged into the website and the completion rate as the proportion who finished the virtual simulation. We conducted our primary analysis using an intention to treat approach. Specifically, we included physicians who did and did not adhere to the requirement to spend an hour on their assigned intervention but restricted our analysis to physicians with outcome data (that is, those who completed the virtual simulation). We assumed that physicians who did not complete would not differ substantially from those who did but then performed a sensitivity analysis in which we explored the effect of departures from that assumption.

We summarized physician characteristics using means (and SD) for continuous variables and proportions (%) for categorical variables.

Adherence, usability, and likeability

We measured adherence as self reported minutes spent on the intervention, summarized using medians and interquartile ranges. We categorized qualitative feedback about the usability and likeability of the intervention as positive or negative. We compared adherence across interventions using a Kruskal-Wallis test and the usability and likeability across interventions using χ2 tests.

Outcome assessment

We first evaluated participants’ performance on each trauma case based on a review of their disposition decisions (transfer, admit, discharge). We categorized patients who died before a disposition decision as “transferred” as we could not predict what the physician would have done given a successful resuscitation and wanted to give him/her the benefit of the doubt. We then calculated each group’s proportion of under-triage (defined as the number of patients not transferred to a trauma center divided by total number of severely injured patients who should have been transferred to a trauma center).46 To be consistent with our statistical analysis plan, we treated the proportion of under-triage as continuous and compared the effects of the intervention (primary outcome), cognitive load (secondary outcome), and the interaction of these factors on under-triage using a two way analysis of variance. In response to the recommendation of an independent statistical reviewer, we completed a post hoc analysis in which we treated under-triage as binomial and used a Poisson regression model with robust standard errors to test these effects.

We included all participants who had outcome data (that is, they completed the outcome assessment tool (simulation) within four weeks of exposure to their assigned treatment). In sensitivity analyses, we tested three different imputation scenarios to explore the potential bias introduced in our effect estimates based on types of non-random missingness. In scenario 1, we imputed worse than cohort average scores for missing physician outcomes in the game arm, assuming they performed like physicians in the education arm. In scenario 2, we imputed better than cohort average scores for missing physician outcomes in the educational arm, assuming they performed like physicians in the game arm. In scenario 3, we imputed worse than cohort average scores for missing physician outcomes in the game arm and better than cohort average scores for missing physician outcomes in the education arm.

We also performed sensitivity analyses to determine the effect of excluding participants who worked at both level I/II trauma centers and non-trauma centers, because they might have different heuristics to physicians who only ever worked at non-trauma centers, and participants who experienced usability issues with the interventions. Finally, we excluded from the analysis cases in which the patient died in the emergency department to see if changing our definition of the outcome would affect our estimate of the effects of the interventions.

In exploratory analyses, defined post hoc, we further assessed the relation between triage decisions and patient representativeness, defined as injuries fitting or not fitting the archetype; adherence, measured as time spent on the intervention (in thirds); and likeability, measured as whether the participant reported liking the intervention. Again we used analysis of variance (ANOVA) and Poisson regressions to test the associations between predictors and outcome measures.

Duration of treatment effect

As during the main trial, we scored each participant’s responses to the simulation, summarized triage decisions at the group level, and compared the effects of the intervention on under-triage using both ANOVA (primary six month reassessment outcome) and Poisson regression analyses (post hoc).

All statistical analyses were conducted with Stata 13.0 (Statacorp, TX).

Human subjects and power calculation

We registered the trial and the follow-up study on clinicaltrials.gov (NCT02857348; NCT03138304). We planned the six month reassessment after initiating the trial, based on our receipt of supplemental funding, and therefore registered the follow-up study as a second trial.

We used Cohen’s method of estimating power for behavioral trials and assumed a 70% completion rate.47 We planned to perform a two way analysis of variance and predicted the mean proportion of under-triage and standard deviation based on results from prior work. This calculation resulted in a plan to recruit 368 physicians, which would give us 80% power to detect an 8-12% (moderate-large) difference in performance between the two intervention groups at a significance level of 0.05.

For the six month outcome study, we used a similar strategy to plan the sample size (albeit assuming a 60% response rate) and estimated that recruiting 200 physicians would give us 80% power to detect an 8% (moderate) difference in performance between the two intervention groups at a significance level of 0.05.

Patient involvement

No patients were involved in setting the research question or the outcome measures, nor were they involved in developing plans for design or implementation of the study. No patients were asked to advise on interpretation or writing up of results. Results of the trial will be made available to all participants via clinicaltrials.gov as well as by email notification.

Results

Participant characteristics

We enrolled 368 physicians in the trial between 16-17 October 2016. Of these, 324 physicians logged into the website (88%), and 297 (81%) completed the outcome assessment portion of the study protocol by 14 November 2016, when the study closed (fig 3). The mean age of physicians completing the protocol was 39.9 (SD 8.9). Of those who took part, 283 (96%) had completed a residency in emergency medicine, 207 (70%) had received certification in advanced trauma life support (table 1), and 36 (12%) currently worked in a level I/II trauma center as well as at a non-trauma center.

Fig 3
Fig 3

Screening, randomization, and analysis. In total, 297 (81%) physicians completed the simulations during the initial trial and 122 (61%) completed the simulations during the follow-up study

Table 1

Characteristics of participating physicians in study of effect of video game versus traditional educational apps on triage decisions in simulated trauma cases. Figures are numbers (percentage) unless stated otherwise

View this table:

In May 2017, we recruited a random sample of 100 physicians from each intervention arm from the 297 who completed the trial and enrolled 142 (71%) in the six month outcome assessment. Of these, 122 (61%) completed the outcome assessment tool for a second time by the time that study closed on 15 June 2017 (fig 3).

Triage decision making

Table 2 shows the effect sizes with confidence intervals and significance levels, and table 3 shows the relative risks. Physicians randomized to receive game based education (n=149) under-triaged fewer severely injured patients than physicians exposed to the didactic educational program (n=148) (316/596 (0.53) v 377/592 (0.64); mean difference 0.11, 95% confidence interval 0.05 to 0.16; P<0.001). The main effect of cognitive load was not significantly related to under-triage and did not interact significantly with intervention assignment. The effect of the intervention on performance did not change when we excluded from the analysis physicians who worked at level I/II trauma centers or cases in which the patient died.

Table 2

Assessment of triage decision making by physicians randomized to video game versus traditional educational apps based on educational programs on simulated trauma cases with analyses of variance

View this table:
Table 3

Assessment of triage decision making by physicians randomized to video game versus traditional educational apps based on educational programs on simulated trauma cases with Poisson regression models

View this table:

When we limited the analysis to the cases most likely to resemble the physicians’ archetype of a severely injured patient (those we had designated as “representative”), exposure to the intervention did not affect triage. When we analyzed decisions for cases less likely to resemble the physicians’ archetype (those we had designated as “non-representative”), however, we found that exposure to game based training did reduce the rate of under-triage compared with exposure to didactic education (186/298 (0.63) v 239/296 (0.81); mean difference 0.18, 95% confidence interval 0.11 to 0.25; P<0.001).

Table 4 shows the sensitivity analysis in which we tested the possible influence of missing data on our results. Regardless of imputation assumption, performance among physicians exposed to the game based education remained significantly higher than among physicians exposed to didactic education.

Table 4

Sensitivity analysis to test effect of missing outcome data in study of effect of video game versus traditional educational apps based on educational programs with analyses of variance

View this table:

Adherence, usability, and likeability of the interventions

Physicians reported spending similar amounts of time with the game (90 minutes; range 30-240; interquartile range 60-120) as on the educational apps (90 minutes; range 45-300; interquartile range 65-120; P=0.06).

Physicians in the game arm more often noted usability problems (30%) than physicians in the didactic education arm (8%, P<0.001). One specific problem, experienced by several participants during the first week of the trial, was a programming error in Night Shift that prevented play after about 75-90 minutes. The gaming company provided an update, via the Apple Store, for the second week of the trial. Many physicians, however, did not download the update and therefore encountered the error.

Physicians who used the game were also less likely to describe their intervention as enjoyable (40%) than physicians who used the educational apps (91%, P<0.001). Physicians who provided positive feedback about the game described the adventure as engaging; those who provided negative feedback described it distracting or annoying (table 5). In contrast, physicians who provided positive feedback about the didactic educational intervention described the apps as useful and accessible; those who provided negative feedback described them as “superficial” or “remedial.”

Table 5

Adherence, usability, and likeability of video game versus traditional educational apps. Figures are numbers (percentage) unless stated otherwise

View this table:

Physicians who spent more time on their assigned intervention had better performance on the triage simulation (tables 2 and 3). Exclusion of physicians who reported usability issues did not alter the effect of the intervention on performance. Reported enjoyment of the intervention was unrelated to performance on the simulation.

Duration of treatment effect

As shown in tables 2 and 3,six months after completing their intervention, physicians who used the video game under-triaged fewer patients on the virtual simulation compared with physicians who used the didactic education program (146/256 (0.57) v 172/232 (0.74); mean difference 0.17, 95% confidence interval 0.09 to 0.25; P<0.001).

Discussion

Principal findings

Exposure to a theoretically based video game changed the behavior of a convenience sample of physicians compared with those exposed to a traditional educational program. The change in behavior was concentrated on the cases we had designated as “non-representative” or least likely to evoke the common archetypes used to classify patients. The game exerted an effect on behavior despite the fact that physicians in the game arm were less likely to describe the intervention as usable or enjoyable.

We hypothesized that the game would recalibrate physicians’ representativeness heuristics by changing their archetypes of patients with minor and severe injuries. We predicted that this change would manifest as a differential response to cognitive load. We found, however, that cognitive load did not affect physicians in either arm of our study. One explanation is that the experimental manipulation of cognitive load did not work. Given that the same manipulation affected performance in prior research, we speculate that the interventions interacted with the load manipulation, inducing a ceiling effect. An alternate explanation is that the game changed behavior through a mechanism other than heuristics. For example, the format could have convinced physicians of the need to transfer injured patients to trauma centers.

Players exposed to the game did not uniformly enjoy the experience. Physician dissatisfaction could reflect the well described observation from the video game literature that people have preferences for different genres of game (such as puzzle games).48 Age could also play a role in physicians’ reactions to Night Shift. Younger physicians, with greater tolerance for games used for training purposes, might represent a more suitable target population for this type of intervention.49 Finally, usability issues with the game probably also affected enjoyment. The game itself was a proof of concept prototype, without the full production values of commercially available games that participants might have come to expect.

Design decisions for the intervention reflected our belief that effective interventions compensate for deficiencies in the experience-feedback loop by immersing or engaging the user in the training task.17 We speculated that enjoyment would offer a means to that end and chose our mechanism (narrative) and delivery platform (video games) accordingly. We found that while adherence influenced performance, however, enjoyment did not. We therefore conclude that though enjoyment is one pathway to engagement, it is not the only one.

Strengths and limitations

Our study had several limitations. First, we selected participants attending a national conference; if they differ from the overall population of practicing emergency medicine physicians this would affect external validity. Use of a convenience sample would have affected both arms equally, however, rendering the results internally valid. Second, the simulation included only 10 cases with an enriched base rate of severe injuries, potentially introducing bias and precluding the assessment of individual physician performance.350 Case volume imposes a well known barrier to reliable and valid estimation of individual physician performance.51 One solution is to create instruments that focus on conditions of interest.52 Another is to aggregate responses to assess group level performance.53 We have previously validated our use of simulation to measure performance by comparing the responses of emergency medicine physicians on the simulation with their practice patterns, finding that key decisions (such as acquisition of radiologic studies, disposition) match.45 Third, physicians exposed the video game might have had an unfair advantage when completing the virtual simulation. We designed the game and simulation, however, with different objectives in mind (engagement versus assessment). As a result, the two products included different mechanics and interfaces (video 1 and video 2). In addition, both groups of physicians reviewed the same tutorial and completed a non-trauma case before beginning the trauma cases, further reducing any carryover effects. Readers who review the trial registration website will note a difference in our terminology regarding the primary outcome measure. In the trial registration, we used “under-triage rate” whereas here we use “proportion of under-triage.” Though the literature on trauma triage uses these terms interchangeably, in the clinical trials literature, “rate” connotes events per time period, whereas proportion does not. Our use of rate and proportion interchangeably reflects imprecision in our language and not an attempt to manipulate outcome reporting.

Conclusions and policy implications

Trauma triage exemplifies the complexity and importance of diagnostic decisions made under time pressure and uncertainty. Severely injured patients treated at trauma centers have better outcomes than patients treated at non-trauma centers, including a 25% reduction in mortality, less disability at discharge, less pain at one year, and increased rates of returning to work.545556 About 55-80% of patients with severe injuries who present initially to non-trauma centers, however, are not transferred to a higher level of care, contributing to 30 000 preventable deaths each year.21235758 Clinical practice guidelines in trauma instruct physicians to triage patients based on a history, physical exam, and chest and pelvic radiographs—ideally as rapidly as possible.4659 In other words, physicians must make their decisions quickly and with incomplete information. Additionally, most physicians have relatively little experience with severely injured patients: physicians working at a non-trauma center evaluate 1000 patients for every one with severe injuries.60 These conditions make it extremely challenging to learn appropriate triage. Existing interventions, which emphasize physician knowledge of and attitudes towards the clinical practice guidelines, do not adequately deal with the challenges faced by physicians at non-trauma centers.

To address this, we developed a novel intervention that combined video game technology with narrative engagement to recalibrate physician heuristics and tested its efficacy in reducing diagnostic errors in simulated trauma triage. Our results suggest that narrative based video games have the potential to influence physician behavior, although the real world implications remain unclear.

What is already known on this topic

  • Strategies designed to change physician decision making have had limited success

  • No interventions exist to improve physician heuristics—the intuitive judgments that drive much of medical decision making

What this study adds

  • In this randomized clinical trial, physicians exposed to a video game intervention were more likely to follow clinical practice guidelines in the triage of simulated trauma patients than physicians exposed to a traditional educational program

  • A theoretically grounded video game intervention has the potential to modify physician behavior, although the magnitude of the effect and real world effectiveness remain uncertain

  • Key limitations include our use of a convenience sample of physicians and the use of a virtual simulation as the outcome assessment tool

Acknowledgments

We thank Jesse Schell, Michal Kacziewski, Jared Mason, Alex Pizzini, Lucy Gouvin, Gabe Yu, and other members of the team at Schell Games for developing Night Shift; Christy Steele, Mia Yoon, and Andrew Cupp for their help with recruitment; Dan Ricketts for help with programming; and the many physicians at the University of Pittsburgh who participated in play testing the video game and the physicians who participated in the trial.

Footnotes

  • Contributors: All authors were responsible for study concept and design. DM drafted the manuscript and is guarantor. All authors critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content read and approved the final manuscript.

  • Funding: This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health through grants DP2 LM012339 (DM) and NHLBI-K08-HL122478 (DJW). RAND Center for Gaming provided funds to recruit physicians for the six month assessment. The funding agencies reviewed the study but played no role in its design or in the collection, analysis or interpretation of data. Schell Games performed work for hire and does not retain any property rights to Night Shift.

  • Competing interests: All the authors have completed the ICMJE uniform disclosure form at www.icmje.org/coi_disclosure.pdf and declare no support from any organization for the submitted work other than those listed above; no financial relationships with any organizations that might have an interest in the submitted work in the previous three years, and no relationships or activities that could appear to have influenced the submitted work.

  • Ethical approval The University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board approved the study and its follow up (PRO16070572).

  • Data sharing: A full dataset of physician-level data and statistical code is available from the corresponding author at mohand@upmc.edu, contingent on approval from the Institutional Review Board at the University of Pittsburgh. Consent was not obtained for data sharing but the presented data are anonymized and risk of identification is low.

  • Transparency: The lead author affirms that the manuscript is an honest, accurate and transparent account of the study being reported, and follows the CONSORT guidelines for the reporting of clinical trials. No important aspects have been omitted, and any discrepancies from the study as planned have been explained.

This is an Open Access article distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial (CC BY-NC 4.0) license, which permits others to distribute, remix, adapt, build upon this work non-commercially, and license their derivative works on different terms, provided the original work is properly cited and the use is non-commercial. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/.

References

View Abstract