Acupuncture for chronic headache in primary care: large, pragmatic, randomised trial
BMJ 2004; 328 doi: https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.38029.421863.EB (Published 25 March 2004) Cite this as: BMJ 2004;328:744
All rapid responses
Vickers, et al, deny that they even said, "The NHS should consider
further funding of acupuncture." That is literally true. Instead, at
separate three points in the paper, they said "Expansion of NHS
acupuncture services should be considered" and included headache, chronic
headace and migraine as specific indications. In there response, the
authors have therefore made a distinction without a difference.
Regardless, the authors may have demonstrated that for a small
subgroup of patients, doing something (acupuncture of some sort) in
addition to standard care seems to be better than doing nothing extra.
This is not surprising, in light of what is known about such things as
treatment responses, demand characterisitics of caregiver interactions,
etc. As for banjo lessons, I personally need them to improve my technique.
However, I make no claims for their health benefits and I am not
suggesting that anyone else should pay for them.
David Ramey, DVM
Competing interests:
None declared
Competing interests: No competing interests
Our trial has clearly raised a great deal of comment. We will try to
address the issues raised by each respondent. A number of correspondents
seem to have misunderstood the question we were seeking to answer in this
trial. This was a pragmatic rather than explanatory trial. The research
question was ‘Does acupuncture make a worthwhile difference to primary
care patients with headache?’, not ‘Is acupuncture better than a placebo?’
The intervention was one which could easily be applied in the NHS, and
used a group of acupuncturists with common professional background and
training, while allowing them judgment in precisely what intervention they
applied. We believe that we have shown that this intervention is
effective and that the conclusions are directly generalisable to NHS
primary care.
Dr Klimek complains that we claimed lower resource use (e.g. GP
visits) in the acupuncture group despite lack of statistical significance.
Whilst this point is well taken, our statement is correct as written.
Moreover, we think that statistical significance has a questionable role
as regards resource use. Our analytic strategy was to A) determine whether
acupuncture has clinical effects such as reduced headache; B) if so,
estimate the resource implications of acupuncture. Statistical
significance is an important part of A, but not B, because your decision
is a clinical one. Now you might find out that the resource implications
of a clinical decision might make you change that decision, for example,
if a treatment worked, but was far too expensive. But you don't start by
looking at cost, otherwise you would use ineffective drugs just because
they were cheap.
Klimek also argues that no patients dropped out of the control group
because treatment was ineffective. This is simply because these patients
did not receive on-study treatment and we did not offer them this as an
option when asking about reasons for withdrawal. It is not true that "this
bias was not addressed" as we undertook careful sensitivity analysis as
described on bmj.com. Note that 96% of patients provided post-
randomization data. We can also recommend Klimek consults table 2 to
answer questions about concurrent medication.
We agree with Professor Ernst that pragmatic trials do not stand
alone and require other types of data. Were our study to be the only trial
ever conducted on acupuncture for headache we agree that it would not form
a basis for policy decisions. However, our trial can be put in the context
of a Cochrane review. This review included a total of 11 trials comparing
acupuncture to placebo acupuncture in patients with migraine. Two found no
effects over sham acupuncture, three showed trends in favor of acupuncture
and five trials reported that patients in the acupuncture group did
significantly better than those in the sham acupuncture group. The final
trial reported a positive trend but was judged to be uninterpretable due
to the high drop-out rate.
We would like to assure Dr Ramey that comparison of clinically
reasonable alternatives such as standard care versus standard care plus
new therapy are routine in the medical literature. Ramey claims that
acupuncture is not "cost effective" on the grounds that it "doubled the
cost of care". This is despite our careful analysis of whether acupuncture
was worth the additional costs using standard techniques and varying the
threshold of willingness to pay. Ramey argues that acupuncture might be
less effective than other interventions, and gives banjo lessons as an
example. We completely agree. This is, of course, an argument against any
trial. For example, one could look at the recent topiramate trial and
claim "okay, topiramate was better than placebo. But was it better than
banjo playing?"
We repudiate Ramey's suggestion that we engaged in statistical
sleight-of-hand. Ramey misreads figure 2: he questions the results of the
control group "at 28 weeks", when the X axis is "headache days at
baseline", thus turning his own mistake into a deliberate attempt to
mislead on behalf of the authors. Ramey engages in further ad hominem
argument when claiming that we repeatedly state: "the NHS should consider
further funding of acupuncture" (which we never do) because our goal was
to obtain funding for a therapy. This reduces scientific debate to
accusations about motives, a datum, of course, to which we have no access.
Numerous respondents, including De Prato, Mansfield, Patterson,
Brookan and Brinkman ask, in short, could acupuncture have been a placebo?
Morris puts it well when he points out that "the acupuncture intervention
actually included referral and transportation to a specialist, the
touching of skin with needles, and the penetration of the acupuncture
needles". We agree that, taken alone, our results do not address the
extent to which the effects of acupuncture are due to "penetration of the
acupuncture needles". This is because our trial did not set out to answer
this question. Nonetheless, our trial, like all clinical trials, needs to
be put in the context of other research, such as the Cochrane review cited
above. We do accept the criticism that our choice of words was not always
optimal. We randomised patients to policies of either "use acupuncture" or
"avoid acupuncture" and our conclusions might better have been stated in
terms of "a policy of use acupuncture leads to …." rather than
"acupuncture leads to …". The clinical implications of our study are
unaffected by this semantic point.
Morris is simply incorrect to state that we required powerful
mathematical manipulation to demonstrate differences between groups. Our
main analysis, ANCOVA, is not only absolutely standard, but has been
widely recommended by statistical groups such as the ECH and FDA.
Moreover, as shown in our sensitivity analysis, an unadjusted ttest finds
highly significant differences between groups. Morris (and also Rienks)
is also incorrect to state that we conducted an effectiveness trial before
evidence from an efficacy trial indicated the intervention to be
efficacious. We refer to the results of the Cochrane review, discussed
above. Morris repeatedly describes the effect of the intervention as
"small". We will leave it to patients to decide whether an additional
three weeks per year free of headache is a small improvement.
Ronellenfitsch makes a reasonable point: only patients interested in
acupuncture would take part in the trial, they might be different from
other migraine patients and hence the results may not be applicable to
"every migraine patient". However, we see no reason why we might want to
apply the results of an acupuncture trial to a patient who would not want
to undergo acupuncture.
Ng and Parkinson complain that different treatments were given by
different practitioners. The key point is made by Ng in the assertion
that: "Prescriptions of acupuncture by different methods give
significantly different outcomes". If this is true, then Parkinson would
be correct is asserting that our trial was the equivalent of assessing
"any drug for chronic headache". But Ng does not provide any data or given
an argument to support the claim that the effects of acupuncture are
highly dependent on exactly which one of several reasonable point
prescriptions are used: it is simply an unsubstantiated assertion.
Conversely, we analyzed our data to determine whether there was
heterogeneity between practitioners, that is, whether different
practitioners obtained different results. Our I squared statistic, which
measures how much the observed variation is due to differences in
practitioners and how much due to chance, was zero. This suggests to us
that differences in prescriptions, as well as other aspects of acupuncture
practice, given by well-trained practitioners do not have a large impact
on outcome. We deliberately selected acupuncturists from one “school” of
acupuncture, AACP training. These means we are assessing one particular
group of acupuncturists which is statutorily regulated and which therefore
would meet clinical governance criteria for referral by other NHS
purchasers. If you take the attitude that only a stardardised protocol can
be researched, this would be the least generalisable type of study as not
even individual practitioners work in a completely standardised way.
Ng also claims that lack of a standard acupuncture protocol renders
our study "irreproducible" and therefore "invalid scientifically". This is
untrue. We have a sample of practitioners in the trial. Even if the
effectiveness of different acupuncturists varies, it remains true that a
patient visiting an acupuncturist in the is likely to have results close
to the mean that we reported in the trial. This is basic statistics:
height varies, but the mean height of a sample is likely to be close to
the population mean. Two experiments assessing the effects of X on Y are
expected to have similar results whether X is constant or a sample of X is
made where X varies. Finally on this point, as we remark above, we saw no
evidence that the effects of treatment do vary between practitioners.
Ng has misunderstood my previous paper. I did not claim that the high
proportion of positive results of acupuncture trials conducted in China
were related to methodologic rigor; I merely noted the phenomenon and gave
methodologic rigor, along with several other factors, as a possible
explanation. Ng claims that our "positive results [can be] attributed to
lack of methodologic rigor" but do not state clearly what it is about our
study methodology that makes it likely that our positive results were a
false positive.
Park argues that the difference between groups may have been due to
the disappointment felt by patients allocated to the control group. The
question is ultimately the same as the question whether the acupuncture
was a placebo (see above). Though this is not an unreasonable suggestion,
it is essentially a speculation, against which stand several lines of
evidence. First, headache scores improved in the control group. Second,
the difference between groups was larger than empirical estimates of the
type of bias Park describes. Third, our results were very similar to the
prior placebo-controlled trial of Vincent. Moreover, it might be argued
that being told there is nothing new that can be done (disappointment)
compared to a possible novel intervention (placebo) mirrors closely the
real world of clinical decision making.
Van den Burg suggests that "If a pharmaceutical medicine had been
studied in this way there would be an outburst of indignation in relation
to the conclusions." We can assure Dr Van den Burg that pharmaceuticals
are compared with no treatment controls routinely. Indeed, I work in
oncology and a typical trial compares chemotherapy for metastatic cancer
with "best supportive care" or adjuvant therapy with surgery alone. We
would also like to restate that our results do not stand alone, and must
be put in the context of other placebo-controlled trials of acupuncture.
Schoonman et al. make a large number of remarks. They first claim
that "the observed differences versus control were clinically irrelevant".
As we have already remarked, we will leave that to patients to tell us
whether 22 fewer days with headache pain each year are worth having. They
go on to state that "the control group was doing much worse than to be
expected rather than the acupuncture group doing better". This is very
hard to square with the actual data we presented: patients in the control
experienced a considerable improvement in headache with approximately 30%
reporting clinically relevant improvement during the trial.
The respondents raise doubts over our primary efficacy score as it is
"not recommended by accepted [International Headache Society] guidelines".
The differences between our 6 point measure (adopted to ensure
comparability with the placebo-controlled trial) and the International
Headache Society's four point measure are hardly drastic. In addition, an
unequivocally accepted endpoint is days with headache. The correlation in
our data set between our primary endpoint and days with headache is close
to 0.8 and the results of the two endpoints are highly comparable. So if
Schoonman et al. are suspicious of our primary endpoint, we invite them to
ignore it and use days with headache instead: their conclusions should be
highly similar. For example, they reinterpret our difference between
groups in terms of the maximum on the scale, rather than the mean reported
in the trial. This is equivalent to claiming that a 35% reduction in the
cost of a $20,000 dollar car is "irrelevant" on the grounds that $7,000 is
only 2 or 3% of the cost of the most expensive cars. With respect to the
average headache severity of 1.7, we can assure Schoonman et al. that
patients in the study did reported high scores, 3 - 5, during migraine
attacks.
Schoonman et al make a number of reading errors. They claim that "out
of 9 SF-36 health status scales, only one showed a statistically
significantly improvement". In fact, there were statistically significant
differences for three scales at the primary endpoint of one year, with a
further four showing statistical trends. They state that they were unsure
as to whether patients completed diaries throughout the year or for just
for one week at 12 months. As explicitly stated under "outcome assessment"
patients completed the diary for four weeks at the one year follow-up. The
respondents also suggest that we exclude patients without a migraine
diagnosis from the analysis. Such a subgroup analysis is presented in the
results section.
Andrew Vickers and the study team.
Competing interests:
Andrew Vickers is the first author of the paper
Competing interests: No competing interests
In my critique of Dr. Vickers' paper, I incorrectly read Figure 2. I
apologize for the erroneous comments drawn from that misreading. Rather
than comparing treatment effects at 28 days, the graph appears to show a
mild benefit of acupuncture in a subset of patients, with other patients,
those with very few or very many headaches (28 days per month), appearing
to show no benefit from acupuncture.
I also note that my comments about statistical sleight-of hand might
have been taken as offensive. They were not intended as such. Other
responses amply demonstrate the problems with both the study and the
statistical interpretations therein, comments with which I wholeheartedly
agree.
David Ramey, DVM
Competing interests:
None declared
Competing interests: No competing interests
Properly conducted high quality trials of any healthcare intervention
are a must and properly conducted trials of therapies such as acupuncture
should be funded and conducted more often than is the current norm.
However, in this study it is difficult to assess what about
acupuncture was actually on trial. Clearly, acupuncture needles, gauge
unknown, number unknown, were inserted somewhere, to some depth, for some
time, using some acupuncture technique, for some number of treatments by
12 Physiotherapists in the patients allocated to receive acupuncture. We
also know that those physiotherapists had some training in Traditional
Chinese Medicine (TCM).
What we do not know is, whether the way the acupuncture was given at
each treatment was based upon the western diagnosis or a TCM diagnosis.
The paper does state that treatment was individualised to each patient but
does not report on how such was decided.
The study would therefore seem to be the equivalent of a trial
reporting "Any drug for chronic headache" versus some other form of care!
Would such a trial ever get ethical approval unless powered in such a way
that sub-group analysis of, at minimum, each class of drug could be
undertaken.
It would therefore seem that this trial missed a real opportunity to
make fundamental progress in our understanding about acupuncture. It
should have been powered to allow for at least some level of sub-group
analysis based upon protocoled "needle insertion plans" and or TCM
diagnosis.
Acupuncture in the UK is given by 4 groups of people- 1) those with a
recognised TCM qualification 2) GPs who have undertaken some training in
acupuncture but do not use TCM diagnosis 3) Physiotherapists without
advanced training who do not use TCM diagnosis 4) Physiotherapists with
advanced training who may or may not use a TCM diagnosis. What does each
of these groups learn from this study? I am not sure.
Competing interests:
5 years ago I undertook some consultancy for the British Acupuncture Council
Competing interests: No competing interests
Sir
I think it only fair, and in the public interest, that any
respondents to this paper who have/have had a professional and/or
financial interest in remedies for headaches and migraine that 'compete
with' acupuncture should declare those interests. I have read that
Prof. Ferrari may have received funding from various pharmaceutical
companies including Glaxo Wellcome (1), and Prof. Goadsby has held the
position of Wellcome Senior Research Fellow (2), therefore
pharmaceutical companies that are involved in the production of headache
and migraine remedies.
Regards
John H.
References
(1)http://www.kopzorgen.nl/nieuws/nieuws24.html
(2) http://www.ion.ucl.ac.uk/~headache/staff.html
Competing interests:
I am a practitioner of Traditional Chinese Medicine acupuncture & moxibustion
Competing interests: No competing interests
Dear editors,
Vickers et al conclude that "acupuncture leads to persisting and
clinically relevant benefits for patients with chronic headache"(1). We
compliment the authors for their laudable attempt in conducting such a
complicated pragmatic trial but we are inclined to arrive at opposite
conclusions. We feel that the observed differences versus control were
clinically irrelevant, and that the control group was "doing much worse"
than to be expected rather than the acupuncture group "doing better" .
The primary efficacy score (weekly headache score) is unusual, not
recommended by accepted guidelines(2), and not validated with respect to
reliability, reproducibility and clinical relevance. Its calculation is
not explained unambiguously. As we understand it, headache severity was
assessed four times daily on a 0-5 score. Thus, the maximum weekly
headache score is 7 x 4 x 5 = 140.
The weekly headache score improved after one year from 24.6 to 16.2
in the acupuncture group and from 26.7 to 22.3 in the usual care group.
This is a difference of 4.6 on a total scale of 140 (3.3 %). We are
uncertain as to the clinical relevance of this statistically significant
difference.
The average baseline weekly headache scores were 24.6 and 26.7 and
the (calculated) average weekly number of days with headache is around 4.
From this we would calculate that the maximum average headache severity
per day was 26.7 / 4 days / 4 assessment points = 1.7. This is less than
grade 2, which was defined as "mild headache that can be ignored at
times". Such a low severity is extremely unlikely for migraine
headaches(3)
The lack of a clinically relevant improvement is also reflected in
the minute or even statistically barely significant improvements for the
secondary outcome measures. For example, after one year the difference in
reduction of number of headache days per month is 1.8 days with an average
headache severity of less than grade 2. Is this a clinically relevant
gain? Similarly, out of 9 SF-36 health status scales, only one showed a
statistically significantly improvement.
We are also unsure as to whether the one year primary endpoint
assessment was based on patients continuing to assess their headaches
scores 4 times daily for 12 months, or on a one week assessment at the end
of the year. The first would be highly unlikely, the second is
statistically unsatisfactory.
Another major problem seems to be the lack of a sham procedure in the
control group and the unblinded design with "open randomisation". The
authors describe that suitable patients were actively invited to
participate in this study, but then 50% were randomised to no treatment
other than usual care. In contrast to blinded controlled studies where
patients do not know that they have been assigned to "no treatment", here
patients were fully aware and may have been greatly disappointed,
potentially resulting in a negative placebo effect. This could explain the
remarkably low response in the control group of only 15% at three and 12
months for both the weekly headaches score and the proportion of patients
with at least a 50% reduction in days with headache. This compares
unfavourably with the usual placebo effects of 20-40%(2) found in migraine
prophylaxis studies. The acupuncture responder rate of 30% is also much
lower than is usually seen for active treatments in migraine prophylactic
trials and more in line of what is seen for placebo rates(2).
Finally, we are concerned that 6% of the study patients did not have
migraine, but instead tension-type headache. Because of the fundamentally
different pathophysiological basis of both disorders, this doesn't seem to
help to understand why acupuncture would work in the first place. Why not
excluding these patients from the analysis? The new work places
complementary therapies squarely on the agenda for research, but can in no
way be taken as proof for the effect of acupuncture in the treatment of
migraine. Moreover, it certainly should not be a basis for funding
widespread uncontrolled application of this approach.
Guus G. Schoonman,
Research fellow
Department of Neurology
Leiden University Medical Centre, Leiden, The Netherlands
Natalie J. Wiendels,
Research fellow
Department of Neurology
Leiden University Medical Centre, Leiden, The Netherlands
Peter J. Goadsby,
Professor of Neurology
Institute of Neurology
The National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square London
UK
Michel D. Ferrari,
Professor of Neurology
Department of Neurology
Leiden University Medical Centre, Leiden, The Netherlands
References
1. Vickers AJ, Rees RW, Zollman CE, McCarney R, Smith CM, Ellis N et
al. Acupuncture for chronic headache in primary care: large, pragmatic,
randomised trial. BMJ 2004;328:744.
2. Tfelt-Hansen P, Block G, Dahlof C, Diener HC, Ferrari MD, Goadsby
PJ et al. Guidelines for controlled trials of drugs in migraine: second
edition. Cephalalgia 2000;20:765-86.
3. Goadsby PJ, Lipton RB, Ferrari MD. Migraine- current
understanding and treatment. N.Engl.J.Med. 2002; 346:257-70.
Competing interests:
None declared
Competing interests: No competing interests
Sir
I would be most grateful for research references from Dr Patterson
that support his contention that any person who "visits another human
being for input for a mean 11 visits" will invariably see an improvement
to migraine/chronic headaches that exceeds the results expected from
visits, as made in Vickers et al study, to GPs for standard care.
I could then refer to these to justify telling chronic
migraine/headache sufferers (that I usually benefit with as many
acupuncture & moxibustion treatments as it takes - supported with
dietary and lifestyle advice) who have run the gauntlet of GPs and
neurologists for decades to no avail that another alternative is to seek
"another human being who will provide input for a mean 11 visits" to
attain the elusive improvement.
If that intervention works, as Dr Patterson and other "pragmatic
neurologists" suggest, why do neurologists and GPs insist on prescribing
drug regimes for so long without benefit when 11 visits to a human being
may suffice; and why have I never heard from a patient that they were
advised by their physician to try the "11 visit to a human being"
approach?
Regards
John H.
Competing interests:
None declared
Competing interests: No competing interests
You claim in This week in the BMJ (27/03/04) that the paper by
Vickers et al shows that acupunture is beneficial for chronic headache
disorders. We would take issue with that claim because the design of the
study does not make it clear what was causing the modest effect which the
authors noted. The treated arm of the study received 3 treatments which
were not received by the control arm: these were, first, input from
another human being for a mean of 11 sessions, second, insertion of
needles, and third, insertion of needles in accordance with acupuncture
prescriptions. Both the BMJ and the authors may attribute the effect to
the third treatment and as pragmatic neurologists (who see large numbers
of these patients) we may attribute it to the first, but sadly both these
conclusions are equally speculative because the design of the trial does
not separate them.
Competing interests:
None declared
Competing interests: No competing interests
Sir
I think the design creates an excellent concept for trialling drugs
'in the market place' after the round of various trial phases is complete
- toxicology, RCT etc. and have found a drug safe and effective.
Unlike all modern drugs, acupuncture has been shown to be safe and
effective over millennia so drugs require different protocols at first to
ensure safety - bearing in mind that they are largely resposnible for
modern medicines position as the 3rd or 4th biggest killer and maimer of
mankind - so great caution must be taken before unleashing any into the
public arena.
Once they become accepted for public consumption, the various drugs
could be trialled for efficacy and effectiveness against acupuncture, and
other modalities, as per the Vickers et al concept - pragmatically.
Trialling drugs as acupuncture was trialled in this study -where the
prescription was left to the style, experience and discretion of the
practitioner - we should soon begin to see the realty of primary care
prescribing; it could also reveal trends in prescribing, costs, patient
satsifaction, and many other outcomes and variables in drug prescribing
etc. the like of which we rarely, if ever, realise from current drugs
research methodology.
I'm not sure how popular this would be with pharmaceutical companies
but I think it would be in the public interest.
Regards
John H.
Competing interests:
None declared
Competing interests: No competing interests
Accurate Accupuncture Study
Full review of the study and its respondants may well
have its detractions, but first and final analysis is too
"determine" ... and that is science.
History -on its own -does not bode well for science.
Manipulated, edited and certainly not "scientific", History
is rather similar to any study. (scientific vs. accurate)
For science is funded and funding has a vast web of
implications ... implied and identified.
I ask simply, why not refer to Hundreds of Years of
"studies" and review what has taken place in China
and the far east for ages.
Interview those that have used and participated in
accupuncture for say, over 30 or even, 50 years!
Examine results from say, 300 participating doctors
or heck, 1,000.
Clean up the questionable parts of your "study" with
any "filter" or "table", that makes you feel good.
Ask yourself ... am I in this for Science Data, Medical
Reward, Hope or Healing?
I personally, have not ever been involved with accupuncture.
I have never studied the science of accupuncture.
I have never placed much faith or really considered the benefits.
However, seeing these responses - I am now curious.
Accupuncture is now on my radar ... it is worth a serious look!
Donald Hall
Bear Creek Research, Inc.
A Kentucky 501(c)(3)Non Profit Corporation,
325 Shoreside Drive,
Lexington, KY 40515
Competing interests:
Light Emitting Diode Research
Competing interests: No competing interests