Intended for healthcare professionals

Papers

Self reported health of people in an area contaminated by chromium waste: interview study

BMJ 2000; 320 doi: https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.320.7226.11 (Published 01 January 2000) Cite this as: BMJ 2000;320:11
  1. Peter McCarron (P.McCarron{at}bristol.ac.uk), lecturer in epidemiology and public health medicinea,
  2. Ian Harvey, professor of epidemiology and public healthb,
  3. Robert Brogan, consultant in public health medicinec,
  4. Tim J Peters, reader in medical statisticsa
  1. a Department of Social Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 2PR
  2. b School of Health, Policy and Practice, University of East Anglia, Norwich R4 7TJ
  3. c Greater Glasgow Health Board, PO Box 15329, Glasgow G3 8YZ
  1. Correspondence to: P McCarro
  • Accepted 16 September 1999

Abstract

Objectives: To compare the self reported health of a group of individuals living in an area contaminated by chromium (chromium group) with a group living in an uncontaminated area (control group), and to assess the effects of perception of risk from exposure to chromium on health.

Design: Cross sectional study using the SF-36 validated quality of life questionnaire. Further questions were added to examine the relations between perceptions about living on or near land contaminated with chromium and the effects on self reported health.

Setting: An area contaminated with chromium (Cambuslang, Carmyle, and Rutherglen) and a control area (Barrmulloch and Pollok).

Participants: Residents of an area containing chromium landfill and residents of an uncontaminated control area.

Main outcome measures: Scores on SF-36.

Results: Little difference was found in health scores between the two groups, and only for general health was there a significantly higher score in the chromium group. Health scores for the chromium group were significantly worse across all dimensions for those who believed that chromium adversely affected health. Most of the chromium group (68%) would prefer money to be spent on improving amenities rather than on chromium remediation.

Conclusions: Similar self reported health among residents of the chromium and control groups indicates that there is no evidence of harm to health from exposure to chromium in this setting. Noticeably lower scores in participants who believed chromium to be harmful to health point to the potential importance of perception and possible anxiety. Given the overall greater desire for better amenities rather than remedial action, policy makers and planners should discuss with residents how best to spend resources before instigating expensive cleaning up programmes.

Introduction

Chromium waste was deposited as landfill in the Cambuslang, Carmyle, and Rutherglen area of Glasgow from 1820 to 1968.1 In 1991 six sites in the area—identified as potentially hazardous to health—were cordoned off, and in 1992 there was concern that several conditions, including leukaemia and kidney tumours, had been caused by the chromium waste, up to 20% of which is in the more toxic hexavalent form.

Chromium, the 21st most abundant element in the earth's crust, exists in three states: chromium(0), chromium(III), and chromium(VI).2 Chromium(III), an essential metabolic trace nutrient,3 is noticeably less toxic than chromium(VI), which can cause perforation of the nasal septum,3 lung cancer,4 and skin ulceration.5 Occupational studies, including those from the United Kingdom and the United States, have reported increased risks of illness in chromate workers, which declined after measures were taken to reduce exposure to chromium.68 Two reviews concluded that there is sufficient evidence for the carcinogenicity of chromium in chromate workers. 4 9 In contrast, community studies have largely been negative. In New Jersey, United States, where chromium has been extensively used as landfill, no evidence of excess lung cancer or other clinical effects was found in residents. 10 11

Studies to investigate the health concerns of Glasgow residents were also negative, reporting no increased risk of congenital abnormalities,12 lung cancer,13 or a range of other diseases14 from living on contaminated land compared with living elsewhere in Glasgow. Indeed the incidence of leukaemia actually rose with increasing distance from the main area of contamination.15

Despite these negative findings and the health reassurances issued to the residents, anxieties persisted, partly due to a perceived lack of impartiality of these studies (personal communication). In light of this continuing concern, and as quality of life issues are underresearched in such settings, the Greater Glasgow Health Board undertook to examine the perceived health of residents in areas of chromium landfill. We report the findings of this study.

Methods

A cross sectional survey was performed in a chromium contaminated area and in an uncontaminated control area about 10 km away. The control area was selected on the basis of its broad similarity to the contaminated area in terms of distribution of age, gender, and Carstairs deprivation categories of the residents.16 Individuals aged 16-74 years were surveyed.

Multistage sampling

After piloting, 400 houses from each area were visited by trained interviewers using a multistage sampling procedure. Areas to survey were selected, and systematic sampling of every twentieth house was carried out. The person answering the door (if aged 16-74 years) was interviewed. If this person refused or there was no reply the interviewer moved on to the next eligible house, continuing in this way until the target number of questionnaires was completed. Only one person per house was interviewed. All interviews were carried out between 9 am and 5 pm and were completed in five days.

SF-36 health questionnaire

The SF-36 was used to measure self reported health. This comprises eight dimensions: vitality, general health, bodily pain, physical and social functioning, mental health, and role limitations due to physical and to emotional problems. After completion of the SF-36 all participants were asked about the difficulties they anticipated in moving from the area in which they lived, should they wish to do so. Additionally, residents of the contaminated area were asked whether they would attribute any such difficulties to the contamination, whether they believed that chromium was harmful to health, and how money allocated to the area should be spent. Age, gender, and housing tenure of the participants were recorded.

Table 1.

Characteristics of chromium and control groups. Values are numbers (percentages) unless stated otherwise

View this table:
Table 2.

Associations between the three potential predictors of health and eight dimensions of the SF-36 in the chromium and control groups combined (all scores range from 0 to 100)

View this table:

Statistical analysis and sample size

Means for each dimension were calculated for both groups and compared using unpaired t tests, after considering the distribution of the data. Multivariable regression analyses were then carried out to determine whether there were differences in SF-36 scores between the two groups after controlling for potential sociodemographic confounders. Similar analyses were then performed for the chromium group alone to examine further the effects of perceptions about harm from chromium and how money allocated should be spent.

Using published standard deviations for the SF-3617 a sample size of 800 (400 in each group) was chosen. This conferred a power of 80% with a two tailed significance level of 5% to detect a difference in health score of five points on the 100 point scale, which is considered to be clinically significant.18

Results

After individuals who were not aged 16-74 years were excluded, the overall response rate was 78%. Table 1 shows the age and sex of the two groups. There was a significant difference in age between the two groups, with the chromium group being older. This was adjusted for in later analyses.

Potential predictor variables

In univariable analyses of the two groups combined there is significant heterogeneity across age groups for all dimensions except role emotional, with health tending to decline with age (data not shown). Gender is only significantly associated with mental health and vitality, women showing poorer scores (data not shown). Table 2 shows the associations between each of three potential predictor variables and the eight dimensions of the SF-36. Owner occupiers had the best perceived health and those in rented accommodation generally the worst. Owner occupiers who believed that there would be a definite problem selling their house had lower health scores than those who believed that this was less likely and this was statistically significant for the dimensions of general, social and mental health, and role emotional.

Comparison of chromium and control groups

Table 3 shows the difference between the two groups for each dimension of the SF-36. In the univariable analysis there are small but statistically significant differences between the two groups for the dimensions of bodily pain, general and physical health, and vitality in the direction of better health in the control group. After adjustment for age and gender there was little change in the differences between the two groups but none of the differences was now significant. Further adjustment for the potential confounding of the other three factors measured in both groups resulted in little change, although for general health there was a statistically significant difference—again in the direction of better health in the control group.

Table 3.

Comparison of SF-36 scores between chromium and control groups. Values are means unless stated otherwise

View this table:

Perception of health risk from chromium

Table 4 shows the scores on the SF-36 dimensions in the chromium group alone for those who believed chromium to be harmful and those who thought it was not harmful. Overall, 25% (98/398) of the chromium group believed that chromium was harmful. In the unadjusted analyses there were large and highly statistically significant differences in all dimensions of the SF-36, with those who believed chromium to be harmful having noticeably worse scores than either those who did not believe it was harmful or those (18/398) who did not know (data not shown). These differences persisted and remained highly significant after adjustment for the same sociodemographic variables as above. Overall, individuals who perceived chromium to be harmful to health scored on average at least 16 points lower on the relevant SF-36 dimensions than did those who thought otherwise.

Table 4.

Comparison of SF-36 scores among residents of chromium contaminated land according to perception of whether chromium contaminated land is harmful to health. Values are means unless stated otherwise

View this table:

Views on resource allocation

Participants in the chromium group were also asked how they would like money to be used if it were allocated to their area. Less than one third (121/383) favoured chromium remediation—removal or burial of the chromium waste—the remainder preferring expenditure on local amenities, housing, education, or employment. There are minor non-significant differences in adjusted scores between these two groups, ranging from −1.4 (95% confidence interval −10.4 to 7.6) to 2.6 (−3.0 to 8.3), and the direction of better health is heterogeneous. Adjustment for sociodemographic variables did not affect these results.

Discussion

In this study, multivariable analyses showed that after adjustment for a range of mainly sociodemographic variables there was little difference in scores on the SF-36 between people living on or near land containing chromium and those living elsewhere. Only for general health was the lower score among the chromium group statistically significant, but this difference was not clinically significant.18 In the chromium group, perceived health was worse in those who believed that chromium is harmful to health. Two thirds preferred money to be spent in other areas governing quality of life rather than on chromium remediation.

Study limitations

The drawbacks of this study include its cross sectional nature, which precludes verification of the direction of the associations. It is possible, although we believe unlikely, that chromium induced ill health causes individuals to correctly identify chromium as a health problem. Lack of objective validation of health status from medical records, the possibility of bias owing to awareness of the key hypotheses among participants, and the availability of only comparatively crude data on potential confounders—age groups and overall socioeconomic status of the two groups—may limit the robustness of the findings. As interviews were conducted during the day participants could have been unrepresentative in terms of employment and health status.

Health risks

Although chromium has known toxic effects, our findings are consistent with those from several previous negative investigations of the role of chromium on health in this population.1215 A more recent study on the health effects of living near to landfill sites in Europe showed an increased risk of congenital birth defects (odds ratio 1.63, 1.09 to 2.44) in Glasgow.19 In this study, however, it is likely that contamination of landfill included a wide range of toxins.

Risk perception and health

Despite the overall negative findings, the lower scores in participants who believe that chromium is harmful raises the possibility that knowledge of the history of land use may reduce generic quality of life in the absence of any documented adverse health effects. This points to the importance of perception in mediating reported health status. Other studies have raised similar issues. In one such study respondent “personality variables,” such as hypochondriasis, opinions about waste disposal, and environmental worry were associated with higher prevalences of physical and psychological symptoms. 20 21 Similarly, the Camelford incident, where the water supply in North Cornwall was contaminated by aluminium sulphate, suggested that the perception of normal and benign somatic symptoms may have been heightened by litigation, community action, self appointed experts, consumer opinion polls, media attention, and accusations of a conspiracy.22 The need for a prompt response, taking into account the importance of social and cultural factors, was underlined.22 Such factors include the nature of the public perception of risk. Risk is a social construct, and it is important in evaluating the threat from environmental hazards to introduce more public participation into both risk assessment and risk decision making.23 This makes the decision making process more democratic, improving the relevance and quality of scientific investigation and enhancing the legitimacy and public acceptance of the resulting decisions. In the current setting, a perceived lack of impartiality of investigations into chromium (G Watt, personal communication) highlights the importance of early public participation when evaluating health risks of environmental waste. An irony that underlines the complexity of this situation is that most of the chromium group preferred money to be spent in other areas governing quality of life rather than on chromium remediation.

Conclusions

Landfill sites are a potentially important cause of environmental pollution and ill health and studies are needed to determine the size and nature of such potential risks. Even when detrimental health effects can be ruled out it is vital to determine whether there are other factors that may be responsible for grievances among exposed populations and what measures can be taken to combat these. Although we are unable to completely discount the possibility of poorer health in the chromium group, our findings, in combination with those of earlier studies,1215 are reassuring and important and reinforce the need for early dialogue with communities to debate the real nature of the problems.

What is already known on this topic

Chromium is known to cause physical health problems such as lung cancer, perforation of the nasal septum, and skin ulceration. Most reports of health effects have been in occupational settings

Epidemiological studies have generally been negative, including three studies carried out in a chromium contaminated area of Glasgow. These studies have, however, not been considered to be impartial by residents. Moreover, little research has been carried out into the self reported health of such residents

What this paper adds

Potential exposure to chromium is not associated with poorer self reported health

In individuals who perceive chromium to be harmful to health, however, health scores were lower than those in the rest of the population. Concurrently there was a desire for money to be used to improve amenities in the area in preference to chromium remediation

Early public participation is important when evaluating the health risks from environmental waste

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr John Womerseley and Mr Tom Sinclair of Greater Glasgow Health Board for assistance in choosing an appropriate control area.

Contributors: PMcC contributed to the design, planning, and conduct of the study and participated in data analysis and writing of the paper. He will act as guarantor for the paper. IH participated in data analysis and writing of the paper. RB contributed to the design, planning, and conduct of the study and made critical comments on the first draft. TJP helped with statistical analyses and contributed critically to the final draft of the paper.

Footnotes

  • Funding Greater Glasgow Health Board.

  • Competing interests None declared.

References

  1. 1.
  2. 2.
  3. 3.
  4. 4.
  5. 5.
  6. 6.
  7. 7.
  8. 8.
  9. 9.
  10. 10.
  11. 11.
  12. 12.
  13. 13.
  14. 14.
  15. 15.
  16. 16.
  17. 17.
  18. 18.
  19. 19.
  20. 20.
  21. 21.
  22. 22.
  23. 23.